How the CSI Effect Influences American Jurors

On the set of CSI: Crime Scene Investigation.

Philip Ramey Photography, LLC / Corbis Entertainment / Getty Images

The CSI effect is a belief held primarily among law enforcement personnel and prosecutors that forensic science television dramas influence American jurors to want more forensic evidence to convict defendants of crimes. It's coined from the show CSI: Crime Scene Investigation, which aired for 15 seasons on CBS and still appears in syndicated reruns.

Public Perceptions of Forensic Science

In forensic science television dramas, crime scene investigators collect and analyze evidence, interview suspects, and solve the crime in one hour. Police and prosecutors know this is unrealistic, but they are concerned that the futuristic technology and speedy resolutions viewers see each week shape the public’s expectations of crime solvers.

Television writers and producers do not make their characters live within the time and funding constraints placed on actual forensic scientists. Professionals worry that jurors may acquit guilty defendants because forensic evidence is not presented by the prosecution at trial. Some experts say they witnessed a change in jury interest in forensic evidence once the television shows became popular in the early 2000s.

"Talking about science in the courtroom used to be like talking about geometry—a real jury turnoff. Now ... you can talk to jurors about (scientific evidence) and just see from the looks on their faces that they find it fascinating,” Jury Consultant Robert Hirschhorn told USA Today in 2004.

The CSI Effect on Trials and Convictions

The CSI effect has not been substantiated by empirical research. “While some existing evidence on juror decision-making is consistent with the CSI effect, it is equally plausible that watching CSI has the opposite impact on jurors and increases their tendency to convict,” Tom Tyler, a professor of law and psychology at Yale Law School, said in the Yale Law Review in 2006.

There are instances documented in news stories where jurors specifically request forensic evidence. Even if these instances can be attributed to the CSI effect, they themselves do not empirically prove a widespread phenomenon.

Stories like these do compel prosecutors to outline to juries why certain pieces of evidence do or do not exist in a case. For example, jurors in a murder case may expect to hear ballistics evidence if the murder was committed using a firearm. If the bullets were damaged so that they could not be conclusively matched to the alleged murder weapon, a prosecutor would explain this rather than omitting the ballistics report from the state’s evidence list.

Eastern Michigan University Criminology Professors Gregg Barak, Young Kim, and Donald Shelton conducted research on the opinions of potential jurors in Ann Arbor, Michigan. In the summer of 2006, they set out to find whether those people who watched programs like CSI demanded to see more scientific evidence before they would convict a defendant.

“Although CSI viewers had higher expectations for scientific evidence than non-CSI viewers, these expectations had little, if any, bearing on the respondents' propensity to convict. This, we believe, is an important finding and seemingly very good news for our Nation's criminal justice system: that is, differences in expectations about evidence did not translate into important differences in the willingness to convict,” Shelton wrote about the research for the National Institute of Justice in March 2008.

Shelton said more of a “tech effect” was observed whereby jurors are influenced by advances in technology rather than what they watch on television. As jurors see technological advances in their own lives, they expect forensic science technology to keep up with or outpace consumer technology.